TY - JOUR
T1 - Optimal foraging and physiological responses to the risk of predation
T2 - How fecal cortisol concentrations from trapped Allenby's gerbil (Gerbillus andersoni allenbyi) relate to foraging under the risk of predation
AU - Juliana, Justin R.St
AU - Kotler, Burt P.
AU - Pinshow, Berry
AU - Kronfeld-Schor, Noga
N1 - Funding Information:
We wish to thank Iris Shubert and Royi Gutman for teaching us the hormone analysis procedures. We would also like to thank Lina Caceres, James Wilson, Shomen Muhkerjee, Vijayan Sundararaj, Rachel C. Carson and Adriana Guerrero for help with experiments and observations. The research was funded by United States-Israel Binational Science Foundation grant 1999-109 to BK, and a Mitrani Department of Desert Ecology Student grant. This is paper number 1009 for the Mitrani Department of Desert Ecology.
Funding Information:
We wish to thank Iris Shubert and Royi Gutman for teaching us the hormone analysis procedures. We would also like to thank Lina Caceres, James Wilson, Shomen Muh-kerjee, Vijayan Sundararaj, Rachel C. Carson and Adriana Guerrero for help with experiments and observations. The research was funded by United States-Israel Binational Science Foundation grant 1999-109 to BK, and a Mitrani Department of Desert Ecology Student grant. This is paper number 1009 for the Mitrani Department of Desert Ecology.
Publisher Copyright:
© 2019 by Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands.
PY - 2019/1/1
Y1 - 2019/1/1
N2 - We studied the influence of manipulating predation risk on Allenby's gerbil (Gerbillus andersoni allenbyi) held in a large, outdoor enclosure. We measured giving up densities (GUDs), apprehension, time allocation to foraging, harvest strategy (grab and go (GAG) vs. eat at tray (EAT)), and fecal cortisol concentration. First we established the time necessary for cortisol and corticosterone concentrations to change significantly from baseline after a stressful experience. To do this we collected feces from gerbils 2, 4, 6, or 8 hours after being handled (treatment) or not (control). After 8 h, fecal cortisol, but not corticosterone, concentration was significantly higher in treatment animals. We used the results from the hormone time course experiment to design the predation experiment. We used a dog, trained to harass gerbils, to increase predation risk for the gerbils. We predicted that fecal cortisol concentrations would increase directly in the face of predation risk, or indirectly, due to reduced foraging time because of perceived predation risk that, in turn, leads to increased hunger levels. As predicted, in the presence of a predator, GUDs were higher, time allocation lower, and GAG foraging was used more in treatment animals than in controls, but we found no change in apprehension. There was no difference in cortisol concentration between predator present and no-predator treatments. However, individuals that tended to have higher average fecal cortisol concentrations also tended, on average, to spend more time foraging. This indicates a relationship between stress hormones and optimal foraging. This relationship is potentially causal. While nightly changes in behavior may not be related to stress hormones, over course time scales, stress hormones may be driving gerbils to forage more.
AB - We studied the influence of manipulating predation risk on Allenby's gerbil (Gerbillus andersoni allenbyi) held in a large, outdoor enclosure. We measured giving up densities (GUDs), apprehension, time allocation to foraging, harvest strategy (grab and go (GAG) vs. eat at tray (EAT)), and fecal cortisol concentration. First we established the time necessary for cortisol and corticosterone concentrations to change significantly from baseline after a stressful experience. To do this we collected feces from gerbils 2, 4, 6, or 8 hours after being handled (treatment) or not (control). After 8 h, fecal cortisol, but not corticosterone, concentration was significantly higher in treatment animals. We used the results from the hormone time course experiment to design the predation experiment. We used a dog, trained to harass gerbils, to increase predation risk for the gerbils. We predicted that fecal cortisol concentrations would increase directly in the face of predation risk, or indirectly, due to reduced foraging time because of perceived predation risk that, in turn, leads to increased hunger levels. As predicted, in the presence of a predator, GUDs were higher, time allocation lower, and GAG foraging was used more in treatment animals than in controls, but we found no change in apprehension. There was no difference in cortisol concentration between predator present and no-predator treatments. However, individuals that tended to have higher average fecal cortisol concentrations also tended, on average, to spend more time foraging. This indicates a relationship between stress hormones and optimal foraging. This relationship is potentially causal. While nightly changes in behavior may not be related to stress hormones, over course time scales, stress hormones may be driving gerbils to forage more.
KW - giving up density
KW - glucocorticoids
KW - optimal foraging
KW - predation risk
KW - rodent
KW - stress hormones
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=85062826783&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.1163/22244662-20191044
DO - 10.1163/22244662-20191044
M3 - Article
AN - SCOPUS:85062826783
SN - 1565-9801
VL - 65
SP - 28
EP - 36
JO - Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution
JF - Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution
IS - 1-2
ER -